Duration threshold for identifying speech samples for different phonemes
Keywords:
Duration threshold, identification, classification, speechAbstract
The identification or classification of acoustic objects is important to decide in which way a sound needs to be interpreted and to rate its importance or relevance. In recent studies, it has been shown that the minimal duration of a sound, which is required for a correct identification, could be a useful audiological parameter, e.g. providing information about the hearing ability of a person. In this work, we want to investigate which cues are used by humans to classify a sound correctly as speech. For this purpose, the duration thresholds for the identification of speech samples starting with different phonemes are analyzed for elderly listeners with normal and impaired hearing. To this end, a two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) method was used, where, as an alternative to speech, a noise signal with a matched frequency spectrum was presented. In contrast to previous studies, there were no frequency cues available and we found no correlation to the pure tone average (PTA) or speech understanding in noise. As one main conclusion, the results suggest that humans primarily exploit the temporal envelope (ENV) rather than the temporal fine structure (TFS) for the identification of short speech samples above hearing threshold and without frequency cues.
References
Budathoki , D., Tchorz, J., and O’Beirne , G. (2019). “Duration Thresholds for Identifying Different Sound Types,” 22. DGA Jahrestagung, Heidelberg, Germany, 2019.
Gray, G. W. (1942). “Phonemic Microtomy: The Minimal Duration of Perceptible Speech Sounds,” Speech Monogr., 9(1), 75-90. doi: 10.1080/03637754209390064
Gygi, B., Kidd, G. R., and Watson, C. S. (2004). “Spectral-temporal factors in the identification of environmental sounds,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 115(3), 1252-1265. doi: 10.1121/1.1635840
Hirsh, I. J., and Watson, C. S. (1996). “Auditory psychophysics and perception,” Ann. Rev. Psychol., 47(1), 461-484. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.461
Husstedt, H., Bank, D., and Schinnerl, M. (2019). “Comparison of Two Procedures to Measure the Duration Threshold for Identifying Sound Samples,” 22. DGA Jahrestagung, Heidelberg, Germany, 2019.
Husstedt, H., Wollermann, S., and Tchorz, J. (2018). “Analysis of the Transition of the Automatic Selection of Hearing Aid Programs,” 45th Erlanger Kolloquium, Erlangen, Germany, 2018.
Kaernbach , C. (1991). “Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method,” Percept. Psychophys., 49(3), 227-229. doi: 10.3758/BF03214307
Kollmeier, B., and Wesselkamp, M. (1997). “Development and evaluation of a Ger- man sentence test for objective and subjective speech intelligibility assessment,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 104(2), 2412-2421. doi: 10.1121/1.419624
Lorenzi, C., Gilbert, G., Carn, H., Garnier, S., and Moore, B. (2006). “Speech perception problems of the hearing impaired reflect inability to use temporal fine structure,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 103(49), 18866-18869. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607364103
McDermott, J. H., and Simoncelli, E. P. (2011). “Sound Texture Perception via Statistics of the Auditory Periphery: Evidence from Sound Synthesis,” Neuron, 71(5), 926-940. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.032
Moore, C. J. M. (1984). “Frequency selectivity and temporal resolution in nor- mal and hearing-impaired listeners,” Br. J. Audiol., 19(3), 189-201. doi: 10.3109/03005368509078973
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Be ́dirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., and Chertkow, H. (2005). “The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment,” J. Am. Geriatr. Soc., 53, 695-699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
Pietro, M., Laganaro, and M., Schnider, A. (2016). “Auditory agnosia,” in Neuropsychological Research: A Review, Edited by P. Marien and J. Abutalebi (Psychology Press), chap. 15.
Additional Files
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
a. Authors retain copyright* and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
b. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
c. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
*From the 2017 issue onward. The Danavox Jubilee Foundation owns the copyright of all articles published in the 1969-2015 issues. However, authors are still allowed to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.