Timing of turn taking between normal-hearing and hearing-impaired interlocutors

Authors

  • Anna Josefine Munch Sørensen Department of Health Technology, Technical University of Denmark (DTU), DK- 2800 Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8148-5692
  • Ewen N MacDonald Department of Health Technology, Technical University of Denmark (DTU), DK- 2800 Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6973-2926
  • Thomas Lunner Department of Health Technology, Technical University of Denmark (DTU), DK- 2800 Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark; Eriksholm Research Centre, Oticon A/S, DK-3070 Snekkersten, Denmark

Keywords:

conversational dynamics, floor-transfer offset, hearing-impaired, normal-hearing adaptation, speaking levels, speaking rates, inter-pausal units, overlapping talk, turn-taking, interlocutors

Abstract

Having a conversation requires more resources than just understanding speech. Previous studies of the timing of turn taking in conversations suggest that in order to sustain normal, fluid turn taking, interlocutors have to predict the end of each other’s turns. Thus, while noise and hearing loss should make understanding speech more difficult, it should also reduce the resources available for speech planning and possibly reduce the saliency of cues used to predict turn ends, resulting in delayed and more variable turn taking. We recorded conversations between 12 pairs of native-Danish young normal-hearing (NH) and older hearing-impaired (HI) listeners with mild presbyacusis in quiet and multitalker babble at three levels. The interlocutors conducted a Diapix task, finding differences in two near-identical pictures. Both HI and NH talkers responded more slowly and with more variability with increasing noise level, and the HI with more variability than the NH. We saw indications that the younger NH adopted a more careful communication strategy, likely to ease the effort on their older HI interlocutor, by adapting their speech rates to their interlocutor and overlapping less.

References

Baker, R., and Hazan, V. (2011). “DiapixUK: Task Materials for the Elicitation of Multiple Spontaneous Speech Dialogs,” Behav. Res. Methods, 43(3), 761–70. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0075-y.
Beechey, T., Buchholz, J. M., and Keidser, G. (2018). ”Measuring communication difficulty through effortful speech production during conversation,” Speech Commun., 100, 18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2018.04.007.
de Jong, N. and Wempe, T. (2009). ”Praat script to detect syllable nuclei and measure speech rate automatically,” Behav. Res. Methods, 41, 385-90. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.2.385.
Hadley, L. V., Brimijoin, W. O., and Whitmer, W. M. (2019). ”Speech, movement, and gaze behaviours during dyadic conversation in noise,” Sci. Rep., 9(1), 10451. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46416-0.
Levinson, S. C., and Torreira, F. (2015). “Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language,” Front. Psychol., 6, 731. doi: 10.1038/s41598- 019-46416-0.
Sørensen, A. J. M., Fereczkowski., M, and MacDonald, E. N. (2020). “Effects of noise and L2 on the timing of turn taking in conversation,” Proc. ISAAR, 7, 85-92.
Watson, S., Sørensen, A. J. M., and MacDonald, E. N. (2020). “The effect of conversational task on turn taking in dialogue,” Proc. ISAAR, 7, 61-68.

Additional Files

Published

2020-04-22

How to Cite

Sørensen, A. J. M., MacDonald, E. N., & Lunner, T. (2020). Timing of turn taking between normal-hearing and hearing-impaired interlocutors. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 7, 37–44. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2019-05

Issue

Section

2019/2. Learning from natural sounds