The effect of hearing loss on auditory spatial attention
Resumé
To understand better how hearing loss affects spatial listening abilities in complex multi-talker situations, we investigated auditory spatial attention in 8 older adults with normal audiometric thresholds below 4 kHz and 8 older adults with bilateral sloping sensorineural hearing losses ranging from moderate to severe in the higher frequencies. In a condition with real spatial separation, a target sentence from the Coordinate Response Measure (CRM) corpus was presented from one spatial location and competing sentences from two different locations, with pre-trial cues specifying the target’s identity and location. In a condition with simulated spatial separation, corresponding perceived spatial locations of the target and competitors were achieved through exploitation of the precedence effect. Seven different probability speci cations indicated the likelihood of the target being presented at the three locations (100-0-0, 80-0- 20, 60-0-40, 0-0-100, 20-0-80, 40-0-60, and 50-0-50, respectively for 0°, 45°, and 90° azimuth). As expected, hearing-impaired listeners performed worse than normal-hearing listeners across all listening conditions, but especially in conditions where the location of the target was less certain. For both groups, performance was superior when the target was presented from the more probable location than when it was presented from the less probable location. Implications for clinical practice and future research will be discussed.
Referencer
Bolia, R. S., Nelson, W. D., Ericson, M. A., and Simpson, B. D. (2000). “A speech corpus for multitalker communications research,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107,1065-1066.
Brungart, D. G., and Simpson, B. D. (2007). “Cocktail party listening in a dynamic multitalker environment,” Percept. Psychophys. 69, 79-91.
CHABA (Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics) (1988). “Speech understanding and aging,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 859–895.
Freyman, R. L., Helfer, K. S., McCall, D. D., and Clifton, R. K. (1999). “The role of perceived spatial separation in the unmasking of speech,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 106, 3578-3588.
Kidd, G., Jr., Arbogast, T. L., Mason, C. R., and Gallun, F. J. (2005). “The advantage of knowing where to listen,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 118, 3804-3815.
Li, L., Daneman, M., Qi, J. G., and Schneider, B. A. (2004). “Does the information content of an irrelevant source differentially affect spoken word recognition in younger and older adults?,” J. Exp. Psychol. Human. 30, 1077-1091.
Mondor, T. A., and Zatorre, R. J. (1995). “Shifting and focusing auditory spatial attention,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percep. Perform. 21 387-409.
Noble, W., Byrne, D., and Lepage, B. (1994). “Effects on sound localization of con guration and type of hearing impairment,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 992-1005.
Singh, G., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., and Schneider, B. A. (2008). “The effect of age on auditory spatial attention in conditions of real and simulated spatial separation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 124, 1294-1305.
Spence, C. J., and Driver, J. (1994). “Covert spatial orienting in audition: Exogenous and endogenous mechanisms,” J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 20, 555– 574.
Yderligere filer
Publiceret
Citation/Eksport
Nummer
Sektion
Licens
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
a. Authors retain copyright* and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
b. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
c. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
*From the 2017 issue onward. The Danavox Jubilee Foundation owns the copyright of all articles published in the 1969-2015 issues. However, authors are still allowed to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.