Verbal fluency naming in children with CIs: What can we learn from children with CIs on sensitive periods for language?
Abstract
This study examined lexical retrieval processes as a possible underlying language mechanism responsible for language deficits in some children with cochlear implants (CIs). Lexical retrieval processing was examined using phonological and semantic verbal fluency (VF) naming tasks. In the VF tasks, children were given one minute to generate as many words as they can that begin with a given sound (/t/, /l/, /f/) or that belong to a certain semantic category (animals, food). Twenty children with CIs and twenty age- and IQ-matched normal-hearing (NH) children aged 7-10 participated in this study. Children with CIs generated fewer words on the VF tasks. In addition, qualitative differences were found in the performance of the two groups on these tasks. Children with CI seem to process words at a slower rate compared to NH children. Children with CIs showed significance differences compared to NH children in the phonological VF task on measures of the number of switches and the number of words produced in the first 15 seconds of the task. Age at implantation was significantly correlated with performance on the semantic part of the VF task. Younger implanted children performed better (named more words) on the semantic VF task. These correlations might suggest that early implantation is advantageous for certain aspects of lexical performance. Taken together the data support recent work suggesting that the development of certain aspects of language may have an earlier sensitive period than other linguistic skills.
References
Frith, U., Karin, L., and Frith, C. (1995). “Dyslexia and verbal fluency: More evidence for a phonological deficit,” Dyslexia, 1, 2-11.
Koren, R., Kofman, O., and Berger, A. (2005). “Analysis of word clustering in verbal fluency of school-age children,” Arch. Clin. Neuropsych., 20, 1087-1104.
Le Normand, M.-T., Ouellet, C., and Cohen, H. (2003). “Productivity of lexical categories in French-speaking children with cochlear implants,” Brain Cognition, 53, 257-262.
Nation, K., Marshall, C.M., and Snowling, M.J. (2001). “Phonological and semantic contributions to children’s picture naming skill: Evidence from children with developmental reading disorders,” Lang. Cognitive Proc., 16, 241-259.
Svirsky, M.A., Robbins, A.M., Iler-Kirk, K., Pisoni, D.B., and Miyamoto, R.T. (2000). “Language development in profoundly deaf children with cochlear implants,” Psychol. Sci., 11, 153-158.
Troyer, A.K. (2000). “Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks,” J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsych., 22, 370-378.
Troyer, A.K., Muscovitch, M., and Winocur, G. (1997). “Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults,” Neuropsychology, 11, 138-146.
Wechsler-Kashi, D. (2011). “Lexical processing during naming in children with cochlear implants,” Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, p. 6734.
Wechsler-Kashi, D., Schwartz, R. G., and Cleary, M. (2013). “Lexical processing during naming in children with cochlear implants,” Ear Hearing (under revision).
Weckerly, J., Wulfeck, B., and Reilly, J. (2001). “Verbal fluency deficits in children with specific language impairment: Slow rapid naming or slow to name?” Child Neuropsychol., 7, 142-152.
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
a. Authors retain copyright* and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
b. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
c. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
*From the 2017 issue onward. The Danavox Jubilee Foundation owns the copyright of all articles published in the 1969-2015 issues. However, authors are still allowed to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.