Studies of pitch mechanisms based on perceptual learning

Authors

  • Brian C. J. Moore Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • Hiromitzu Miyazono Department of Administration, Prefectural University of Kumamoto, Tsukide, Kumamoto, Japan

Abstract

Mechanisms of pitch perception were studied using perceptual learning. In one set of studies, subjects discriminated the fundamental frequency (F0) of a target group of harmonics embedded in a background of harmonics with fixed F0. The results were potentially affected by pitch discrimination interference (PDI) and by cues related to pitch pulse asynchrony (PPA) between the target and background. Large learning effects occurred when PPA cues were available. Training was given using a stimulus with cosine-phase harmonics and high harmonics in the target, under conditions where PPA was not useful. Learning occurred, and it transferred to other cosine-phase, but not to random-phase, tones. The learning may reflect improvements in the ability to overcome PDI. In a second set of studies, F0 discrimination was measured for tones with cosine- or random-phase harmonics, bandpass filtered with five harmonics within the passband and presented in threshold-equalizing noise. Groups trained with LOW, MID, or MID-HIGH stimuli (harmonics 1-5, 11-15, or 14-18, respectively) showed learning effects that transferred to other stimuli except HIGH (28-32). A group trained with HIGH stimuli showed no learning effect, suggesting that a different mechanism was used for the HIGH stimuli than for the other stimuli. We propose that the LOW, MID, and MID-HIGH stimuli were discriminated using temporal fine structure information.

References

Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2003). “Pitch discrimination of diotic and dichotic tone complexes: harmonic resolvability or harmonic number?” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 113, 3323-3334.

Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2005). “An autocorrelation model with place dependence to account for the effect of harmonic number on fundamental frequency discrimination,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 117, 3816-3831.

de Cheveigné, A. (2005). “Pitch perception models,” in Pitch Perception. Edited by C.J. Plack, A.J. Oxenham, R.R. Fay, and A.N. Popper (Springer, New York), pp. 169-233.

Gockel, H., Carlyon, R.P., and Plack, C.J. (2004). “Across-frequency interference effects in fundamental frequency discrimination: questioning evidence for two pitch mechanisms,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 116, 1092-1104.

Gockel, H., Carlyon, R.P., and Moore, B.C.J. (2005). “Pitch discrimination interference: The role of pitch pulse asynchrony,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 117, 3860-3866.

Gockel, H.E., Carlyon, R.P., and Plack, C.J. (2009a). “Further examination of pitch discrimination interference between complex tones containing resolved harmonics,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 125, 1059-1066.

Gockel, H.E., Hafter, E.R., and Moore, B.C.J. (2009b). “Pitch discrimination interference: The role of ear of entry and of octave similarity,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 125, 324-327.

Goldstein, J.L. (1973). “An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 54, 1496-1516.

Grimault, N., Micheyl, C., Carlyon, R.P., and Collet, L. (2002). “Evidence for two pitch encoding mechanisms using a selective auditory training paradigm,” Percept. Psychophys., 64, 189-197.

Hartmann, W.M., and Doty, S.L. (1996). “On the pitches of the components of a complex tone,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 99, 567-578.

Hawkey, D.J., Amitay, S., and Moore, D.R. (2004). “Early and rapid perceptual learning,” Nat. Neurosci., 7, 1055-1056.

Houtsma, A.J.M., and Smurzynski, J. (1990). “Pitch identification and discrimination for complex tones with many harmonics,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 87, 304-310.

Jackson, H.M., and Moore, B.C.J. (2013). “The dominant region for the pitch of complex tones with low fundamental frequencies,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 134, 1193-1204.

Meddis, R., and O'Mard, L. (1997). “A unitary model of pitch perception,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 102, 1811-1820.

Miyazono, H., Glasberg, B.R., and Moore, B.C.J. (2010). “Perceptual learning of fundamental frequency (F0) discrimination: Effects of F0, harmonic number, and component phase,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 128, 3649-3657.

Miyazono, H., and Moore, B.C.J. (2009). “Perceptual learning of frequency discrimination for tones with low fundamental frequency: Learning for high but not for low harmonics,” Acoust. Sci. Tech., 30, 383-386.

Miyazono, H., and Moore, B.C.J. (2013). “Implications for pitch mechanisms of perceptual learning of fundamental frequency discrimination: Effects of spectral region and phase,” Acoust. Sci. Tech. (in press).

Moore, B.C.J., Glasberg, B.R., and Peters, R.W. (1985). “Relative dominance of individual partials in determining the pitch of complex tones,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 77, 1853-1860.

Moore, B.C.J., and Ohgushi, K. (1993). “Audibility of partials in inharmonic complex tones,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 93, 452-461.

Moore, B.C.J., Huss, M., Vickers, D.A., Glasberg, B.R., and Alcántara, J.I. (2000). “A test for the diagnosis of dead regions in the cochlea,” Br. J. Audiol., 34, 205-224.

Moore, B.C.J., and Moore, G.A. (2003a). “Discrimination of the fundamental frequency of complex tones with fixed and shifting spectral envelopes by normally hearing and hearing-impaired subjects,” Hear. Res., 182, 153-163.

Moore, B.C.J., Glasberg, B.R., Low, K.-E., Cope, T., and Cope, W. (2006). “Effects of level and frequency on the audibility of partials in inharmonic complex tones,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 120, 934-944.

Moore, B.C.J., Hopkins, K., and Cuthbertson, S.J. (2009). “Discrimination of complex tones with unresolved components using temporal fine structure information,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 125, 3214-3222.

Moore, B.C.J., and Gockel, H. (2011). “Resolvability of components in complex tones and implications for theories of pitch perception,” Hear. Res., 276, 88-97.

Moore, B.C.J. (2012). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 6th Ed. (Brill, Leiden, The Netherlands), pp. 1-441.

Moore, G.A., and Moore, B.C.J. (2003b). “Perception of the low pitch of frequency-shifted complexes,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 113, 977-985.

Plack, C.J., and Oxenham, A.J. (2005). “The psychophysics of pitch,” in Pitch Perception. Edited by C.J. Plack, A.J. Oxenham, R.R. Fay, and A.N. Popper (Springer, New York), pp. 7-55.

Plomp, R. (1964). “The ear as a frequency analyzer,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 36, 1628-1636.

Plomp, R. (1967). “Pitch of complex tones,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 41, 1526-1533.

Plomp, R., and Mimpen, A.M. (1968). “The ear as a frequency analyzer II,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 43, 764-767.

Ritsma, R.J. (1967). “Frequencies dominant in the perception of the pitch of complex sounds,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 42, 191-198.

Schouten, J.F. (1940). “The residue and the mechanism of hearing,” Proc. Kon. Ned. Akad. Wetenschap., 43, 991-999.

Schouten, J.F., Ritsma, R.J., and Cardozo, B.L. (1962). “Pitch of the residue,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 34, 1418-1424.

Wang, J., Baer, T., Glasberg, B.R., Stone, M.A., Ye, D., and Moore, B.C.J. (2012). “Pitch perception of concurrent harmonic tones with overlapping spectra,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 132, 339-356.

Downloads

Published

2013-12-15

How to Cite

Moore, B. C. J., & Miyazono, H. (2013). Studies of pitch mechanisms based on perceptual learning. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 4, 13–24. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2013-02

Issue

Section

2013/1. Basic perceptual studies of training, learning, and generalization