The relationship between stream segregation of complex tones and frequency selectivity
Keywords:
stream segregation, pitch, frequency selectivityAbstract
The discrimination of changes in fundamental frequency (F0) is better for complex tones with low than with high harmonics, perhaps because the low harmonics are spectrally resolved. The reduced frequency selectivity of hearing-impaired (HI) participants may lead to poorer resolution of low and medium harmonics. This may adversely affect F0 discrimination and in turn reduce the extent of perceptual segregation (streaming) of a rapid sequence of complex tones. We assessed how the streaming of complex tones is affected by harmonic rank and whether HI listeners are less able to segregate tones with low and medium harmonics than near normal-hearing (NH) participants. Subjective streaming was assessed for complex tones that were bandpass filtered between 2 and 4 kHz. Harmonic rank was varied by changing the baseline F0 (with differences in F0 from 5 to 11 semitones). Auditory filter shapes were estimated from notched-noise masking using a 2-kHz signal. The auditory filters were wider for the HI than for the NH participants. Streaming decreased with increasing harmonic rank but was similar for the two groups. Streaming scores were not correlated with auditory filter bandwidths. The results suggest that the effects of harmonic rank on streaming cannot be explained in terms of resolvability.
References
Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2005). “An autocorrelation model with place dependence to account for the effect of harmonic number on fundamental frequency discrimination,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 117, 3816-3831. doi: 10.1121/1.1904268
Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2006a). “The relationship between frequency selectivity and pitch discrimination: effects of stimulus level,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 120, 3916-3928. doi: 10.1121/1.2372451
Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2006b). “The relationship between frequency selectivity and pitch discrimination: sensorineural hearing loss,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 120, 3929-3945. doi: 10.1121/1.2372452
Glasberg, B.R., and Moore, B.C.J. (1990). “Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data,” Hear. Res., 47, 103-138.
Grimault, N., Micheyl, C., Carlyon, R.P., Arthaud, P., and Collet, L. (2000). “Influence of peripheral resolvability on the perceptual segregation of harmonic complex tones differing in fundamental frequency,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 108, 263-271. doi: 10.1121/1.429462
Grimault, N., Micheyl, C., Carlyon, R.P., Arthaud, P., and Collet, L. (2001). “Perceptual auditory stream segregation of sequences of complex sounds in subjects with normal and impaired hearing,” Br. J. Audiol., 35, 173-182.
Houtsma, A.J.M., and Smurzynski, J. (1990). “Pitch identification and discrimination for complex tones with many harmonics,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 87, 304-310. doi: 10.1121/1.399297
Madsen, S.M.K., Dau, T., and Moore, B.C.J. (2015). “Effect of harmonic rank on the streaming of complex tones,” Proc. ISAAR, 5, 477-483.
Moore, B.C.J., and Gockel, H. (2002). “Factors influencing sequential stream segregation,” Acta Acust., 88, 320-333.
Moore, B.C.J. (2003). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th Ed. (Emerald, Bingley, UK), pp. 413.
Patterson, R.D. (1976). “Auditory filter shapes derived with noise stimuli,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 59, 640-654. doi: 10.1121/1.380914
Patterson, R.D., Nimmo-Smith, I., Weber, D.L., and Milroy, R. (1982). “The deterioration of hearing with age: frequency selectivity, the critical ratio, the audiogram, and speech threshold,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 72, 1788-1803.
Rosen, S., Baker, R.J., and Darling, A. (1998). “Auditory filter nonlinearity at 2 kHz in normal hearing listeners,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 103, 2539-2550. doi: 10.1121/1.422775
Stone, M.A., Glasberg, B.R., and Moore, B.C.J. (1992). “Simplified measurement of impaired auditory filter shapes using the notched-noise method,” Br. J. Audiol. 26, 329-334. doi: 10.3109/03005369209076655
Vliegen, J., and Oxenham, A. J. (1999). “Sequential stream segregation in the absence of spectral cues,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 105, 339-346. doi: 10.1121/1.424503.
Vliegen, J., Moore, B.C.J., and Oxenham, A. J. (1999). “The role of spectral and periodicity cues in auditory stream segregation, measured using a temporal discrimination task,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 106, 938-945. doi: 10.1121/1.427140
Additional Files
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
a. Authors retain copyright* and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
b. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
c. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
*From the 2017 issue onward. The Danavox Jubilee Foundation owns the copyright of all articles published in the 1969-2015 issues. However, authors are still allowed to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.