The relationship between stream segregation of complex tones and frequency selectivity

Authors

  • Sara M. K. Madsen Hearing Systems, Department of Electrical Engineering, Technical University of Denmark, Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark
  • Torsten Dau Hearing Systems, Department of Electrical Engineering, Technical University of Denmark, Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark
  • Brian C. J. Moore Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Keywords:

stream segregation, pitch, frequency selectivity

Abstract

The discrimination of changes in fundamental frequency (F0) is better for complex tones with low than with high harmonics, perhaps because the low harmonics are spectrally resolved. The reduced frequency selectivity of hearing-impaired (HI) participants may lead to poorer resolution of low and medium harmonics. This may adversely affect F0 discrimination and in turn reduce the extent of perceptual segregation (streaming) of a rapid sequence of complex tones. We assessed how the streaming of complex tones is affected by harmonic rank and whether HI listeners are less able to segregate tones with low and medium harmonics than near normal-hearing (NH) participants. Subjective streaming was assessed for complex tones that were bandpass filtered between 2 and 4 kHz. Harmonic rank was varied by changing the baseline F0 (with differences in F0 from 5 to 11 semitones). Auditory filter shapes were estimated from notched-noise masking using a   2-kHz signal. The auditory filters were wider for the HI than for the NH participants. Streaming decreased with increasing harmonic rank but was similar for the two groups. Streaming scores were not correlated with auditory filter bandwidths. The results suggest that the effects of harmonic rank on streaming cannot be explained in terms of resolvability.

References

Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2003). “Pitch discrimination of diotic and dichotic tone complexes: harmonic resolvability or harmonic number?,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 113, 3323-3334. doi: 10.1121/1.1572146

Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2005). “An autocorrelation model with place dependence to account for the effect of harmonic number on fundamental frequency discrimination,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 117, 3816-3831. doi: 10.1121/1.1904268

Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2006a). “The relationship between frequency selectivity and pitch discrimination: effects of stimulus level,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 120, 3916-3928. doi: 10.1121/1.2372451

Bernstein, J.G., and Oxenham, A.J. (2006b). “The relationship between frequency selectivity and pitch discrimination: sensorineural hearing loss,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 120, 3929-3945. doi: 10.1121/1.2372452

Glasberg, B.R., and Moore, B.C.J. (1990). “Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data,” Hear. Res., 47, 103-138.

Grimault, N., Micheyl, C., Carlyon, R.P., Arthaud, P., and Collet, L. (2000). “Influence of peripheral resolvability on the perceptual segregation of harmonic complex tones differing in fundamental frequency,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 108, 263-271. doi: 10.1121/1.429462

Grimault, N., Micheyl, C., Carlyon, R.P., Arthaud, P., and Collet, L. (2001). “Perceptual auditory stream segregation of sequences of complex sounds in subjects with normal and impaired hearing,” Br. J. Audiol., 35, 173-182.

Houtsma, A.J.M., and Smurzynski, J. (1990). “Pitch identification and discrimination for complex tones with many harmonics,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 87, 304-310. doi: 10.1121/1.399297

Madsen, S.M.K., Dau, T., and Moore, B.C.J. (2015). “Effect of harmonic rank on the streaming of complex tones,” Proc. ISAAR, 5, 477-483.

Moore, B.C.J., and Gockel, H. (2002). “Factors influencing sequential stream segregation,” Acta Acust., 88, 320-333.

Moore, B.C.J. (2003). An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th Ed. (Emerald, Bingley, UK), pp. 413.

Patterson, R.D. (1976). “Auditory filter shapes derived with noise stimuli,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 59, 640-654. doi: 10.1121/1.380914

Patterson, R.D., Nimmo-Smith, I., Weber, D.L., and Milroy, R. (1982). “The deterioration of hearing with age: frequency selectivity, the critical ratio, the audiogram, and speech threshold,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 72, 1788-1803.

Rosen, S., Baker, R.J., and Darling, A. (1998). “Auditory filter nonlinearity at 2 kHz in normal hearing listeners,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 103, 2539-2550. doi: 10.1121/1.422775

Stone, M.A., Glasberg, B.R., and Moore, B.C.J. (1992). “Simplified measurement of impaired auditory filter shapes using the notched-noise method,” Br. J. Audiol. 26, 329-334. doi: 10.3109/03005369209076655

Vliegen, J., and Oxenham, A. J. (1999). “Sequential stream segregation in the absence of spectral cues,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 105, 339-346. doi: 10.1121/1.424503.

Vliegen, J., Moore, B.C.J., and Oxenham, A. J. (1999). “The role of spectral and periodicity cues in auditory stream segregation, measured using a temporal discrimination task,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 106, 938-945. doi: 10.1121/1.427140

Additional Files

Published

2017-12-14

How to Cite

Madsen, S. M. K., Dau, T., & Moore, B. C. J. (2017). The relationship between stream segregation of complex tones and frequency selectivity. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 6, 231–238. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2017-28

Issue

Section

2017/4. Assessment of specific auditory functions and hearing ability