Subjective listening effort and electrodermal activity in listening situations with reverberation and noise
Abstract
Disturbing factors like reverberation or ambient noise can obstruct speech recognition and raise the listening effort needed for communication in daily life. Situations with high listening effort are considered to incur an increased stress for the listener. The aim of this study was to assess listening effort in situations with background noise and reverberation. For this purpose, a subjective scaling of the listening effort, together with the electrodermal activity (EDA) as a measure of the autonomic stress reaction, was used. Ten young normal-hearing (NH) and 17 elderly hearing-impaired (HI) participants listened to sentences from the Oldenburg sentence test in stationary background noise and reverberation. Four listening situations were generated, an easy and a hard one for each of the two disturbing factors, which were related to each other by the Speech Transmission Index (STI). The results of the subjective scaling showed significant differences between the easy and the hard listening situations in both subject groups. However, various analyses of the EDA values indicate differences between the results of the groups. For the NH listeners, similar tendencies were observed both in the subjective results and the physiological EDA data. For the HI listeners, these effects in the EDA data were less pronounced.
References
Critchley, H.D. (2002). “Electrodermal Responses: What happens in the Brain,” The Neuroscientist, 8, 132-142.
Goldstein, D.S. and Kopin, I.J. (2007). “Evolution of concepts of stress,” Stress, 10, 109-120.
Gramann, K. and Schandry, R. (2009). Psychophysiologie: Körperliche Indikatoren psychischen Geschehens, Fourth Edition (Beltz PVU, Winheim).
Houtgast, T. and Steeneken, H.J.M. (1985). “A review of the mtf concept in room acoustics and its use for estimating speech intelligibility in auditoria,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 77, 1069-1077.
Larsby, B., Hällgren, M., Lyxell, B., and Arlinger, S. (2005). “Cognitive perfor-mance and perceived effort in speech processing tasks: Effects of different noise backgrounds in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects,” Int. J. Audiol., 44, 131-143.
Luts, H., Eneman, K., Wouters, J., Schulte, M., Vormann, M., Buechler, M., Dillier, N., Houben, R., Dreschler, W.A., Froehlich, M., Puder, H., Grimm, G., Hohmann, V., Leijon, A., Lombard, A., Mauler, D., and Spriet, A. (2010). “Multicenter evaluation of signal enhancement algorithms for hearing aids,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 127, 1491-1505.
Mackersie, C.L. and Cones, H. (2011). “Subjective and psychophysiological indexes of listening effort in competing-talker task,” J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 22, 113-122.
Mackersie, C.L., PacPhee, I.X., and Heldt, E.W. (2015). “Effects of hearing loss on hart rate variability and skin conductance measured during sentence recognition in noise,” Ear Hearing, 36, 145-154.
Rennies, J., Holube, I., and Verhey, J.L. (2013). “Loudness of speech and speech-like signals,” Acta Acust. United Acust. 99, 268-282.
Rennies, J., Schepker, H., Holube, I., and Kollmeier, B. (2014). “Listening effort and speech intelligibility in listening situations affected by noise and reverberation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 136, 2642-2653.
Schandry, R. (1989). Lehrbuch der Psychophysiologie: Körperliche Indikatoren psychischen Geschehens, Second Edition (Psychologie Verlags Union, München-Weinheim).
Schepker, H., Haeder, K., Rennies, J., and Holube, I. (2015). “Listening effort and speech intelligibility in reverberation and noise for hearing-impaired listeners,” Int. J. Audiol. (submitted).
Wagener, K., Brand, T., and Kollmeier, B. (1999). “Entwicklung und Evaluation eines Satztests für die deutsche Sprache I: Optimierung des Oldenburger Satztests,” Z. Audiol., 38, 4-15.
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Authors who publish with this journal agree to the following terms:
a. Authors retain copyright* and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License that allows others to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.
b. Authors are able to enter into separate, additional contractual arrangements for the non-exclusive distribution of the journal's published version of the work (e.g., post it to an institutional repository or publish it in a book), with an acknowledgement of its initial publication in this journal.
c. Authors are permitted and encouraged to post their work online (e.g., in institutional repositories or on their website) prior to and during the submission process, as it can lead to productive exchanges, as well as earlier and greater citation of published work (See The Effect of Open Access).
*From the 2017 issue onward. The Danavox Jubilee Foundation owns the copyright of all articles published in the 1969-2015 issues. However, authors are still allowed to share the work with an acknowledgement of the work's authorship and initial publication in this journal.