Is cochlear gain reduction related to speech-in-babble performance?

Authors

  • Kristina DeRoy Milvae Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
  • Joshua M. Alexander Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
  • Elizabeth A. Strickland Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA

Abstract

Noisy settings are difficult listening environments. With some effort, individuals with normal hearing are able to overcome this difficulty when perceiving speech, but the auditory mechanisms that help accomplish this are not well understood. One proposed mechanism is the medial olivocochlear reflex (MOCR), which reduces cochlear gain in response to sound. It is theorized that the MOCR could improve intelligibility by applying more gain reduction to the noise than to the speech, thereby enhancing the internal signal-to-noise ratio. To test this hypothesized relationship, the following measures were obtained from listeners with clinically normal hearing. Cochlear gain reduction was estimated psychoacoustically using a forward masking task. Speech-in-noise recognition was assessed using the QuickSIN test (Etymotic Research), which generates an estimate of the speech reception threshold (SRT) in background babble. Results were surprising because large reductions in cochlear gain were associated with large SRTs, which was the opposite of the hypothesized relationship. In addition, there was a large range for both cochlear gain reduction and SRT across listeners, with many individuals falling outside of the normal SRT range despite having normal hearing thresholds. 

References

Alexander, J.M. and Masterson, K.M. (2015). “Effects of WDRC release time and number of channels on output SNR and speech recognition,” Ear Hearing, 36, e35-e49.

Aguilar, E., Eustaquio-Martin, A., and Lopez-Poveda, E.A. (2013). “Contralateral efferent reflex effects on threshold and suprathreshold psychoacoustical tuning curves at low and high frequencies,” J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 14, 341-357.

Backus, B.C. and Guinan, J.J., Jr. (2006). “Time-course of the human medial olivocochlear reflex,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 119, 2889-2904.

Bernstein, J.G.W. (2012). “Controlling signal-to-noise ratio effects in the measurement of speech intelligibility in fluctuating maskers,” Proc. ISAAR 2011: Speech Perception and Auditory Disorders. 3rd International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, Nyborg, Denmark. Eds. T. Dau, M.L. Jepsen, T. Poulsen, and J.C. Dalsgaard. ISBN 978-87-990013-3-0 (The Danavox Jubilee Foundation, Copenhagen), pp. 33-44.

Bidelman, G.M. and Bhagat, S.P. (2015). “Right-ear advantage drives the link between olivocochlear efferent ‘antimasking’ and speech-in-noise listening benefits,” NeuroReport, 26, 483-487.

Cooper, N.P. and Guinan, J.J., Jr. (2006). “Efferent-mediated control of basilar membrane motion,” J. Physiol., 576, 49-54.
de Boer, J. and Thornton, A.R.D. (2008). “Neural correlates of perceptual learning in the auditory brainstem: Efferent activity predicts and reflects improvement at a speech-in-noise discrimination task,” J. Neurosci., 28, 4929-4937.

de Boer, J., Thornton, A.R.D., and Krumbholz, K. (2012). “What is the role of the medial olivocochlear system in speech-in-noise processing?” J. Neurophysiol., 107, 1301-1312.

Delano, P.H., Elgueda, D., Hamame, C.M., and Robles, L. (2007). “Selective attention to visual stimuli reduces cochlear sensitivity in chinchillas,” The J. Neurosci., 27, 4146-4153.

Fletcher, H. and Galt, R.H. (1950). “The perception of speech and its relation to telephony,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 22, 89-151.

Ghitza, O. (1988). “Auditory neural feedback as a basis for speech processing,” ICASSP, 88, 91-94.

Giraud, A.L., Garnier, S., Micheyl, C., Lina, G., Chays, A., and Chery-Croze, S. (1997). “Auditory efferents involved in speech-in-noise intelligibility,” NeuroReport, 8, 1779-1783.

James, A.L., Harrison, R.V., Pienkowski, M., Dajani, H.R., and Mount, R.J. (2005). “Dynamics of real time DPOAE contralateral suppression in chinchillas and humans,” Int. J. Audiol., 44, 118-129.

Jennings, S.G., Strickland, E.A., and Heinz, M.G. (2009). “Precursor effects on behavioral estimates of frequency selectivity and gain in forward masking,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 125, 2172-2181.

Kawase, T., Delgutte, B., and Liberman, M.C. (1993). “Antimasking effects of the olivocochlear reflex. II. Enhancement of auditory-nerve response to masked tones,” J. Neurophysiol., 70, 2533-2549.

Killion, M.C., Niquette, P.A., Gudmundsen, G.I., Revit, L.J., and Banerjee, S. (2004). “Development of a quick speech-in-noise test for measuring signal-to-noise ratio loss in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 116, 2395-2405.

Krull, V. and Strickland, E.A. (2008). “The effect of a precursor on growth of forward masking,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 123, 4352-4357.

Kumar, U.A. and Vanaja, C.S. (2004). “Functioning of olivocochlear bundle and speech perception in noise,” Ear Hearing, 25, 142-146.

Levitt, H. (1971). “Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 49, 467-477.

Maison, S., Micheyl, C., and Collet, L. (2001). “Influence of focused auditory attention on cochlear activity in humans,” Psychophysiology, 38, 35-40.

May, B.J., Budelis, J., and Niparko, J.K. (2004). “Behavioral studies of the olivocochlear efferent system – Learning to listen in noise,” Arch. Otolaryngol., 130, 660-664.

Messing, D.P., Delhorne, L., Bruckert, E., Braida, L.D., and Ghitza, O. (2009). “A non-linear efferent-inspired model of the auditory system; matching human confusions in stationary noise,” Speech Commun., 51, 668-683.

Nelson, D.A., Schroder, A.C., and Wojtczak, M. (2001). “A new procedure for measuring peripheral compression in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 110, 2045-2064.

Roverud, E. and Strickland, E.A. (2010). “The time course of cochlear gain reduction measured using a more efficient psychophysical technique,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 128, 1203-1214.

Roverud, E. and Strickland, E.A. (2014). “Accounting for nonmonotonic precursor duration effects with gain reduction in the temporal window model,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 135, 1321-1334.

Strickland, E.A. (2001). “The relationship between frequency selectivity and overshoot,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 109, 2062-2073.
Wagner, W., Frey, K., Heppelmann, G., Plontke, S.K., and Zenner, H.-P. (2008). “Speech-in-noise intelligibility does not correlate with efferent olivocochlear reflex in humans with normal hearing,” Acta Oto-Laryngol., 128, 53-60.

Winslow, R.L. and Sachs, M.B. (1987). “Effect of electrical stimulation of the crossed olivocochlear bundle on auditory nerve response to tones in noise,” J. Neurophysiol., 57, 1002-1021.

Yasin, I., Drga, V., and Plack, C.J. (2014). “Effect of human auditory efferent feedback on cochlear gain and compression,” J. Neurosci., 34, 15319-15326.

Downloads

Published

2015-12-15

How to Cite

Milvae, K. D., Alexander, J. M., & Strickland, E. A. (2015). Is cochlear gain reduction related to speech-in-babble performance?. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 5, 43–50. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2015-05

Issue

Section

2015/1. Characterizing individual differences in hearing loss