Perceptual effects of ambisonics on room auralization

Authors

  • Sylvain Favrot Centre for Applied Hearing Research, Department of Electrical Engineering, Technical University of Denmark, DK-2800 Lyngby, Denmark
  • Jörg Buchholz Centre for Applied Hearing Research, Department of Electrical Engineering, Technical University of Denmark, DK-2800 Lyngby, Denmark

Abstract

Higher-order Ambisonics (HOA) has been widely used for loudspeaker-based reproduction of static and moving sound sources from arbitrary directions. When Ambisonics is used to reproduce anechoic sound sources, it is known that the achievable directionality is limited and coloration artifacts are introduced. However, it is unclear to what extend HOA affects auditory performance measures when it is applied to reproduce reverberant sounds. In order to investigate this issue, a loudspeaker-based room auralization (LoRA) system was utilized, which was recently developed at the Centre for Applied Hearing Research. Within the LoRA system, HOA is used in different ways to auralize the individual parts of a simulated room impulse response (i.e. direct sound, early re ections, and reverberation). In order to study the effect of HOA on the auditory processing of reverberant sounds, rst, the effect of the applied Ambisonic order on speech intelligibility in noise was investigated and compared to results obtained with “single loudspeaker” auralization. Second, a distance perception experiment was performed to evaluate the entire LoRA processing in comparison to recorded binaural auralization. For these two experiments, listener performance in HOA-generated virtual environments was similar to that of the reference case which represented the “real” environment.

References

Blauert, J. (1997). Communication acoustics (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA). Bradley, J. S., Sato, H., and Picard, M. (2003). “On the importance of early reflections for speech in rooms,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 3233-3244.

Dalenbäck, B. I. (1996). “Room acoustic prediction based on a uni ed treatment of diffuse and specular reflection,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 899–909.

Daniel, J., Nicol, R., and Moreau, S. (2003). “Further investigations of high order Ambisonics and wave eld synthesis for holophonic sound imaging,” AES 114th Convention, preprint 5788.

Favrot, S., and Buchholz, J. M. (2009a). “Validation of a loudspeaker-based room auralization system using speech intelligibility measures,” AES 126th Convention, preprint 7763.

Favrot, S., and Buchholz, J. M. (2009b). “Distance perception in loudspeaker-based room auralization,” AES 127th Convention, preprint 7854.

Favrot, S., and Buchholz, J. M. (2010). “LoRA – A loudspeaker-room auralization system,” Acta Acustica/Acustica 96, 364-375.

Gerzon, M. A. (1973). “Periphony - with-height sound reproduction,” J. Audio Eng. Soc. 21, 2-10.

Rindel, J. H., Nielsen, G. B., and Christensen, C. L. (2010). “ODEON website,” www.odeon.dk, last viewed 10/01/2010.

Shirley, B., Kendrick, P., and Churchill, C. (2007). “The effect of stereo crosstalk on intelligibility: comparison of a phantom stereo image and a central loudspeaker source,” J. Audio Eng. Soc. 55, 852-863.

Wagener, K., Josvassen, J. L., and Ardenkjaer, R. (2003). “Design, optimization and evaluation of a Danish sentence test in noise,” Int. J. Audiology 42, 10-17.

Zahorik, P. (2002). “Auditory display of sound source distance,” Proc. Int. Conf. on Auditory Display 326-332.

Zahorik, P., Brungart, D. S., and Bronkhorst, A. W. (2005). “Auditory distance perception in humans: A summary of past and present research,” Acta Acustica/ Acustica 91, 409-420.

Additional Files

Published

2009-12-15

How to Cite

Favrot, S., & Buchholz, J. (2009). Perceptual effects of ambisonics on room auralization. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 2, 165–174. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2009-17

Issue

Section

2009/2. Perceptual measures and models of spatial hearing