Induction of auditory perceptual learning

Authors

  • Beverly A. Wright Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Knowles Hearing Center, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA

Abstract

Performance on many perceptual tasks improves with practice even in adults, indicating that our sensory systems are not rigid but rather can be changed through experience. My co-workers and I have been investigating the factors that induce perceptual learning on auditory skills. We have evidence that two key requirements for perceptual improvement to occur across days are performance of the task to be learned and a sufficient amount of training per day. Beyond these core requirements, we also have documented that perceptual training can be made more efficient by not exceeding the required amount of daily training and by replacing a subset of the training trials with stimulus exposure alone. The elements of successful training regimens provide insights into perceptual-learning mechanisms. A greater knowledge of these mechanisms will lead to more effective training strategies to help restore perceptual skills in people with perceptual disorders as well as to enhance those skills in people with normal perception.

References

Aberg, K.C., Tartaglia, E.M., and Herzog, M.H. (2009). “Perceptual learning with chevrons requires a minimal number of trials, transfers to untrained directions, but does not require sleep,” Vis. Res., 49, 2087-2094.

Ahissar, M., and Hochstein, S. (1993). “Attentional control of early perceptual learning,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 90, 5718-5722.

Ahissar, M., and Hochstein, S. (2004). “The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning,” Trends Cogn. Sci., 8, 457-464.

Banai, K., Ortiz, J.A., Oppenheimer, J.D., and Wright, B.A. (2010). “Learning two things at once: Constraints on the acquisition phase of perceptual learning,” Neurosci., 165, 436-444.

Bao, S., Chang, E.F., Woods, J., and Merzenich, M.M. (2004). “Temporal plasticity in the primary auditory cortex induced by operant perceptual learning,” Nat. Neurosci., 7, 974-981.

Cohen, J. (1988) “Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences,” in The concepts of power analysis (Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc, Hillsdale).

Hauptmann, B., and Karni A. (2002). “From primed to learned: the saturation of repetition priming and the induction of long-term memory,” Cogn. Brain Res., 13, 313-322.

Huyck, J.J., and Wright, B.A. (2011). “Late maturation of auditory perceptual learning,” Develop. Sci., 14, 614-621.
Levitt H (1971). “Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 49, 467-477.

McGaugh, J.L. (2000) “Memory-A century of consolidation,” Science, 287, 248-251.

Mednick, S.C., Nakayama, K., Cantero, J.L., Atienza, M., Levin, A.A., Pathak, N., and Stickgold, R. (2002). “The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration,” Nat. Neurosci., 5, 677–681.

Mossbridge, J.A., Fitzgerald, M.B., O’Connor, E.S., and Wright, B.A. (2006). “Perceptual-learning evidence for separate processing of asynchrony and order tasks,” J. Neurosci., 26, 12708-12716.

Ortiz, J.A., and Wright, B.A. (2010). “Differential rates of consolidation of conceptual and stimulus learning following training on an auditory skill,” Exp. Brain Res., 201, 441-451.

Polley, D.B., Steinberg, E.E., and Merzenich, M.M. (2006). “Perceptual learning directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences,” J. Neurosci., 26, 4970-4982.

Rammsayer, T.H. (1994). “Effects of practice and signal energy on duration discrimination of brief auditory intervals,” Percept .Psychophys. 55, 454-464.

Roth, D.A., Amir, O., Alaluf, L., Buchsenspanner, S., and Kishon-Rabin, L. (2003). “The effect of training on frequency discrimination: generalization to untrained frequencies and to the untrained ear,” J. Basic Clin. Physiol. Pharmacol., 14, 137-150.

Savion-Lemieux, T., and Penhune, V.B. (2005) “The effects of practice and delay on motor skill learning and retention,” Exp. Brain. Res., 161, 423-431.

Seitz, A.R., and Watanabe, T. (2005) “A unified model for perceptual learning,” Trends. Cogn. Sci., 9, 329-334.

Wright, B.A., and Sabin, A.T. (2007). “Perceptual learning: How much daily training is enough?” Exp. Brain Res., 180, 727-736.

Wright, B.A., Sabin, A.T., Zhang, Y., Marrone, N., and Fitzgerald, M.B. (2010). “Enabling perceptual learning by alternating practice with sensory stimulation alone,” J. Neurosci., 30, 12868-12877.

Zach, N., Kanarek, N., Inbar, D., Grinvald, Y., Milestein, T., and Vaadia, E. (2005). “Segregation between acquisition and long-term memory in sensorimotor Learning,” Eur. J. Neurosci., 22, 2357–2362.

Downloads

Published

2013-12-15

How to Cite

Wright, B. A. (2013). Induction of auditory perceptual learning. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 4, 1–11. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2013-01

Issue

Section

2013/1. Basic perceptual studies of training, learning, and generalization