The occlusion effect and its reduction

Authors

  • Jorge Mejia National Acoustic Laboratories, Sydney, Australia; Cooperative Research Centre for Hearing, Australia
  • Harvey Dillon National Acoustic Laboratories, Sydney, Australia; Cooperative Research Centre for Hearing, Australia

Abstract

The occlusion effect is a major problem for hearing aid wearers. If unsolved, it makes their own voice sound un-natural, and if solved it can severely limit the gain that can be achieved without feedback oscillation. It appears to be caused by the jaw vibrating to a much greater magnitude than the other bones of the skull, because of the much lower weight of the jaw. Jaw vibration then causes one wall of the ear canal to vibrate with respect to the opposing wall. A new solution to the occlusion effect is to add a second, inward-looking microphone to a hearing aid. This microphone senses the occlusion-induced sound pressure within the ear canal, inverts it, and feeds it back to the receiver to partially cancel the occlusion-induced sound pressure. The processing operates according to well-established rules of negative feedback, an important component of which is ltering to ensure that gain around the loop is less than unity for any frequency at which phase shifts cause the feedback to be positive. Occlusion reduction of 15 dB in the target frequency range is achievable, and as this is the same magnitude as the occlusion effect for the average hearing aid wearer, active occlusion reduction can completely remove occlusion for the average wearer, despite having little or no venting. Because the active occlusion reduction also cancels any sound entering the ear canal via any small vent or leakage path, the processing allows electronic ampli cation to be provided, even with negative gains if desired. This substantially widens the range of frequencies over which directional microphones and adaptive noise suppression can be effective.

References

Benjamin, C. K. (1982). “Automatic Control System,” Fourth edition, Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Beranek, L. (1986). “Acoustics,” Revised edition, Am. Inst. of Physics.

Carle, R., Laugesen. S., and Nielsen, C. (2002). “Observations on the relations among occlusion effect, compliance, and vent size,” J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 13(1), 25-37.

Dillon, H., Birtles, G., and Lovegrove, R. (1999). “Measuring the outcomes of a national rehabilitation program: Normative data for client oriented scale of improvement (COSI) and the hearing aid user’s questionnaire (HAUQ),” J. Am. Acad. Audiol., 10, 67-79.
Dillon, H. (2001). “Hearing Aids,” Sydney, Boomerang Press.

Hakansson, B., Carlsson, P., and Tjellstron, A. (1986). “The mechanical point impedance of the human head, with and without skin penetration,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 80, 1065-1075.

Hitay, O. (1999). “Introduction to feedback control theory,” CRC Press, Inc.

Killion, M. C. (1988). “The “hollow voice” occlusion effect,” in Hearing Aid Fitting: Theoretical and Practical Views, 13th Danavox Symposium, Copenhagen, edited by J. Hartvig Jensen, 231-241.

MacKenzie, K., Browning, G. G., and McClymont, L. G. (1989). “Relationship between earmould venting, comfort and feedback,” Br. J. Audiol. 23(4), 335-337.

Mueller, H. G., Bright, K. E., and Northern, J. L. (1996). “Studies of the hearing aid occlusion effect,” Seminars in Hearing, 17(1), 21-32.

Pirzanski, C. Z. (1998). “Diminishing the occlusion effect: clinician/manufacturer-related factors,” Hearing J., 51(4), 66-78.

Slager, G. E. C. (1997). “Mathematical model of the human jaw system simulating static biting and movements after unloading,” J. Neurophysiol., 78(6), 3222-3233.

Additional Files

Published

2007-12-15

How to Cite

Mejia, J., & Dillon, H. (2007). The occlusion effect and its reduction. Proceedings of the International Symposium on Auditory and Audiological Research, 1, 459–470. Retrieved from https://proceedings.isaar.eu/index.php/isaarproc/article/view/2007-45

Issue

Section

2007/5. Recent concepts in cochlear-implant and hearing-aid processing